探花直播 of Cambridge - US Air Force /taxonomy/external-affiliations/us-air-force en Detect. Lock on. Intercept. 探花直播remarkable hunting ability of the robber fly /research/news/detect-lock-on-intercept-the-remarkable-hunting-ability-of-the-robber-fly <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/image5holco04credittrevorwardill.jpg?itok=2bP3T8f4" alt="" title="Credit: None" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p> 探花直播robber fly Holcocephala is a relatively small fly 鈥 at 6mm in length, it is similar in size of the average mosquito. Yet it has the ability to spot and catch prey more than half a metre away in less than half a second 鈥 by comparison to its size, this would be the equivalent of a human spotting its prey at the other end of a football pitch. Even if the prey changes direction, the predator is able to adapt mid-air and still catch its prey.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>An international team led by researchers from the 探花直播 of Cambridge was able to capture this activity by tricking the fly into launching itself at a fake prey 鈥 in fact, just a small bead on a fishing line. This enabled the team to witness the fly鈥檚 remarkable aerial attack strategy. Their findings are published today in the journal <em>Current Biology</em>.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>To read more, see <a href="https://cambridge-uni.medium.com/detect-lock-on-intercept-12d3ea2dae1e">our article on Medium</a>.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><em>See the world through the eyes of a robber fly in the Plant and Life Sciences Marquee at the Cambridge Science Festival, Saturday 18 March 2017.</em></p>&#13; &#13; <p><em><strong>Reference</strong><br />&#13; Wardill, TJ et al. A novel interception strategy in a miniature robber fly with extreme visual acuity; Current Biology; 9 March 2017; DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.01.050</em></p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>A small fly the size of a grain of rice could be the Top Gun of the fly world, with a remarkable ability to detect and intercept its prey mid-air, changing direction mid-flight if necessary before sweeping round for the kill.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-media field-type-file field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><div id="file-122212" class="file file-video file-video-youtube"> <h2 class="element-invisible"><a href="/file/122212"> 探花直播Robber Fly 鈥 Top Gun of the fly world</a></h2> <div class="content"> <div class="cam-video-container media-youtube-video media-youtube-1 "> <iframe class="media-youtube-player" src="https://www.youtube-nocookie.com/embed/ukiTGsvFP1Y?wmode=opaque&controls=1&rel=0&autohide=0" frameborder="0" allowfullscreen></iframe> </div> </div> </div> </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; 探花直播text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div> Thu, 09 Mar 2017 16:44:24 +0000 cjb250 186032 at Killer flies: how brain size affects hunting strategy in the insect world /research/features/killer-flies-how-brain-size-affects-hunting-strategy-in-the-insect-world <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/features/160204insect-brain-sizes-comparedcredit-sam-fabian.jpg?itok=a_DfO0zJ" alt="" title="Size comparison of robber fly, dragon fly, killer fly (left to right), Credit: Sam Fabian" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>As in economics, there is a law of diminishing returns in neuroscience 鈥 doubling the investment going in doesn鈥檛 equal double the performance coming out. With a bigger brain comes more available resources that can be allocated to certain tasks, but everything has a cost, and evolution weighs the costs against the benefits in order to make the most efficient system.</p> <p>鈥淟arger brains are specialised for high performance, so there鈥檚 a definite advantage to being bigger and better,鈥 says Professor Simon Laughlin of the Department of Zoology, whose research looks at the cellular costs associated with various neural tasks. 鈥淏ut since most animals actually have very small brains, there must also be advantages to being small.鈥 Indeed, there is strong selection pressure to have the minimum performance required in order to survive and it鈥檚 not biologically necessary to be the best, only to be better than the nearest competitor.</p> <p>So does size matter? Do small insects with relatively few neurons have the same capabilities as much larger animals? 鈥淲hen an animal is limited, is it because their neural system just can鈥檛 cope? Or is it because they鈥檙e actually optimised for their particular environment?鈥 asks Dr Paloma Gonzalez-Bellido from Cambridge鈥檚 Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience.<img alt="" src="/sites/www.cam.ac.uk/files/inner-images/160204_holco_square_credit-sam-fabian.jpg" style="width: 250px; height: 250px; float: right;" /></p> <p>With funding from the US Air Force, Gonzalez-Bellido is studying the hunting behaviours of various flying insects 鈥 from tiny killer flies, slightly larger robber flies to large dragonflies 鈥 to determine how their visual systems influence their attack strategy, and what sorts of trade-offs they have to make in order to be successful.</p> <p>Dragonflies are among the largest flying insects, and hunt smaller insects such as mosquitoes while patrolling their territories. They have changed remarkably little in the 300 million years since they evolved 鈥 most likely because they are so well optimised for their particular environmental niche.</p> <p><img alt="" src="/sites/www.cam.ac.uk/files/inner-images/160204_dragon-fly_credit-sam-fabian.jpg" style="width: 250px; height: 250px; float: right;" /></p> <p>鈥淥ther researchers have found that dragonflies are capable of doing complex things like internally predicting what their body is going to do and compensating for that 鈥 for instance, if they鈥檙e chasing a target and turn their wings, another signal will be sent to turn their head, so that the target stays in the same spot in their visual field,鈥 says Gonzalez-Bellido. 鈥淏ut are smaller animals, such as tiny flies, capable of achieving similarly complex and accurate feats?鈥</p> <p>Gonzalez-Bellido also studies the killer fly, or <em>Coenosia attenuata</em>. These quick and ruthless flies are about four millimetres long, and will go after anything they think they can catch 鈥 picky eaters they are not. However, the decision to go after their next meal is not as simple as taking off after whatever tasty-looking morsels happen to fly by. As soon as a killer fly takes off after its potential prey, it exposes itself and runs the risk of becoming a meal for another killer fly.</p> <p>To help these predacious and cannibalistic flies eat (and prevent them from being eaten), they need to fly fast and to see fast. Insects see at speeds much higher than most other animals, but even for insects, killer flies and dragonflies see incredibly fast, at rates as high as 360 hertz (Hz) 鈥 as a comparison, humans see at around 60 Hz.</p> <p>鈥淔or prey animals, the most important thing is to get out of the way quickly 鈥 it doesn鈥檛 matter whether they know exactly what鈥檚 coming, just that it doesn鈥檛 catch them,鈥 says Gonzalez-Bellido. 鈥淧redators need to be both fast and accurate in their movements if they鈥檙e going to be successful 鈥 but for very small predators such as insects, there are trade-offs that need to be made.鈥</p> <p>By making the 鈥榩ixels鈥 on their photoreceptors (the light-sensitive cells in the retina) as narrow as possible, killer flies trade sensitivity for resolution. In bright light, they see better than their similar-sized prey, the common fruit fly. However, the cap on sensitivity and resolution imposed upon killer flies by their tiny eyes means that they can only see and attack things that fly close by.</p> <p>While dragonflies, with their larger eyes and better resolution, can take their time and use their brain power to calculate whether a prey is suitable for an attack, killer flies attack before they鈥檝e had a chance to determine whether it鈥檚 something they can actually catch, subdue or eat 鈥 or they risk missing their prey altogether. Once a killer fly gets relatively close to its potential prey, it has to decide whether to keep going or turn back 鈥 this is one of the trade-offs resulting from evolving such a tiny visual system.</p> <p><img alt="" src="/sites/www.cam.ac.uk/files/inner-images/160204_killer-fly_credit-sam-fabian.jpg" style="width: 250px; height: 250px; float: right;" /></p> <p>In the early 2000s, Laughlin determined the energy efficiency of single neurons, by estimating the numbers of ATP molecules 鈥 the molecules that deliver energy in cells 鈥 used per bit of information coded. To do this he compared photoreceptors in various insects. Laughlin and his colleagues found that photoreceptors are like cars 鈥 the higher the performance, the more energy they require, and costs rise out of proportion with performance. 鈥淔or any system, whether it鈥檚 in a tiny insect or a large mammal, you don鈥檛 want something which is over-engineered, because it鈥檚 going to cost more,鈥 says Laughlin. 鈥淪o what鈥檚 the root of inefficiency, and how did nature evolve efficient nerve cells from the bottom up?鈥</p> <p>Researchers in the Department of Engineering are taking the reverse approach to answer questions about how the brain works so efficiently by looking at systems from the top down. 鈥淚f you reverse engineer an animal鈥檚 behavioural strategy by asking how an animal would solve a task under specific constraints and then work out the optimal solution, you鈥檒l find it鈥檚 often the case that animals are pretty close to optimal,鈥 says Dr Guillaume Hennequin, who looks at how neurons work together to produce behaviour.</p> <p>Hennequin studies how brain circuits are wired in such a way that they become optimised for a task: how primates such as monkeys are able to estimate the direction of a moving object, for example. 鈥淗ow brain circuits generate optimal interpretations of ambiguous information received from imperfect sensors is still not known,鈥 he says. 鈥淐oping with uncertainty is one of the core challenges that brains must confront.鈥</p> <p>Different animals come up with their own solutions. Both dragonflies and killer flies have systems that are optimal, but optimal in their own ways. It鈥檚 beneficial for killer flies to be so small, since this gives them high manoeuvrability, enabling them to catch prey that turns at speed. Dragonflies are much bigger, and can do things that killer flies can鈥檛, but their size means they can鈥檛 turn or stop on a dime, like a killer fly can.</p> <p>鈥淏y answering some of the questions around efficiency in brain circuits, large or small, we may be able to understand fundamental principles about how brains work and how they evolved,鈥 says Laughlin.</p> <p><em>Inset images: top to bottom: robber fly, dragon fly, killer fly; credit: Sam Fabian.</em></p> </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Cambridge researchers are studying what makes a brain efficient and how that affects behaviour in insects.</p> </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">When an animal is limited, is it because their neural system just can鈥檛 cope? Or is it because they鈥檙e actually optimised for their particular environment?</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Paloma Gonzalez-Bellido </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/" target="_blank">Sam Fabian</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Size comparison of robber fly, dragon fly, killer fly (left to right)</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br /> 探花直播text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p> </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div> Tue, 09 Feb 2016 09:10:36 +0000 sc604 166652 at