ֱ̽ of Cambridge - personality /taxonomy/subjects/personality en Musical preferences unite personalities across the globe /stories/musical-preferences-unite-personalities-worldwide <div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Research involving more than 350,000 participants from six continents has found that links between musical preferences and personality are universal. ֱ̽study suggests that music could play a greater role in overcoming social division.</p> </p></div></div></div> Thu, 10 Feb 2022 08:00:00 +0000 ta385 229791 at Gentrification changes the personality make-up of cities in just a few years /research/news/gentrification-changes-the-personality-make-up-of-cities-in-just-a-few-years <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/mercedes-alvarez-i5dmg-9hiza-unsplash1.jpg?itok=VdGll_tX" alt="Mural in the SoHo district of New York, one of the US cities featured in the study." title="Mural in the SoHo district of New York, one of the US cities featured in the study., Credit: Mercedes Álvarez via Unsplash " /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>Rising house prices may change the personality make-up of US cities within a few years, with residents becoming increasingly open-minded – not just as wealthier people move in, but also among longer-term locals.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>This is according to a ֱ̽ of Cambridge-led study of almost two million people in the US living across 199 cities. Psychologists tracked annual personality scores over nine years (2006 to 2014) and compared the data to local housing markets.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽researchers found that just a $50 rise in a city’s average housing prices saw the characteristic of 'openness' increase significantly* among residents (relative to other US cities). Openness is one of five major personality traits, and captures levels of curiosity and creativity.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Changes in housing prices were associated with shifts in ‘Openness’ in US cities where the trait was already very high compared to the rest of the country, such as New York and Chicago.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Even in San Francisco, long famous for its open-minded residents, citywide levels of ‘Openness’ rose sharply over the nine-year period as average housing costs jumped by almost $200.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Previous research has shown that house prices largely reflect the prevalence of 'social amenities': from restaurants to theatres, sports venues, green spaces and well-performing schools.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽study’s authors point out that such amenities appeal to open-minded people, and argue that greater access to these facilities helps to drive “local cultures of openness”.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Theorists going back to Karl Marx have argued that economic development drives national shifts in personality and culture,” said Dr Friedrich Götz, lead author of the study now published in the <a href="https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2022-13889-009">journal <em>American Psychologist</em></a>. (Pre-print available here: <a href="https://psyarxiv.com/7wt89/">https://psyarxiv.com/7wt89/</a>)</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“We can now see how rapidly those changes occur in smaller and more nimble communities such as cities, where major cultural shifts can be experienced in just a few short years, rather than decades or centuries,” he said.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽trait of ‘Openness’ is strongly associated with liberal votes and attitudes as well as entrepreneurial activity. It is also linked to socioeconomic status: the desire and freedom to explore new experiences can be a side effect of sufficient wealth and security.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Data modelling was used to discount socio-economic status at an individual level by incorporating education and self-reported 'social class' into calculations. ֱ̽team also separated out the cities from overall national and state-level trends for ‘Openness’.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽psychologists then investigated two main ways that house prices and associated amenities are linked to personality shifts within urban populations. </p>&#13; &#13; <p>'Selective migration' is when certain types of people move to cities or neighbourhoods, having been attracted by the local culture. 'Social acculturation' refers to changes in individual personalities, in this case through exposure to greater opportunities – from arts scenes to diverse cuisines – and more open-minded neighbours.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Researchers attempted to disentangle these effects by separating out the data into 'established populations' – those who lived in a city prior to 2006, the earliest year in the study – and 'newcomer populations': those who relocated between 2006 and 2014.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽study showed both factors at play: rising housing costs predicted a significant increase in ‘Openness’ among both established and newcomer populations in cities right across the US.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Substantial personality shifts within cities can and do occur within a couple of years,” said Dr Jason Rentfrow, the study’s senior author from Cambridge’s Department of Psychology and fellow of Fitzwilliam College.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Cities are magnets for certain types of people, even as they become increasingly unaffordable – particularly for young people. These cultural changes may go on to affect the personality of long-term residents.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Research has shown that openness is related to economic resilience, creative capital and innovation, as well as liberal politics and arts,” said co-author Tobias Ebert. “ ֱ̽geographical clustering of personality reinforces existing social and economic differences across the country. We can see this reflected in contemporary political divisions.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽study author’s point to US cities such as Pittsburgh: once home to blue-collar industry, it was devastated by the manufacturing collapse in the 1970s, yet by the mid-1980s had become a hub for medicine and higher education – leading to rising housing costs and a cultural transformation that continues to attract the young and open-minded.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽researchers analysed all major personality traits, such as neuroticism and extroversion, as part of the study – but only openness was connected to housing costs. Importantly, the price of housing appears to drive local culture, but not vice versa: increases in openness did not predict more expensive housing.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> </p>&#13; &#13; <p><img alt="" src="/sites/www.cam.ac.uk/files/inner-images/figure-1_crop.jpg" style="width: 800px; height: 498px;" /></p>&#13; &#13; <p><u>Notes:</u></p>&#13; &#13; <ul>&#13; <li>*An annual $50 increase in overall city-wide housing costs saw a city’s level of ‘openness’ rise by a .17 of standard deviation in the following year relative to other cities.</li>&#13; <li> ֱ̽1,946,752 survey participants were all aged between 15 and 70, and provided a valid postal code. ֱ̽data for housing costs incorporated changes in both homeowner costs and the total rent paid by tenants. </li>&#13; <li>Dr Friedrich Götz worked on the study while completing his PhD at the ֱ̽ of Cambridge. He has since taken up a position at the ֱ̽ of British Columbia.</li>&#13; </ul>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Massive study of almost two million US residents reveals rising housing costs may drive increases in “openness” of character among both long-term and new inhabitants of a city.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Substantial personality shifts within cities can and do occur within a couple of years</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Jason Rentfrow</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="https://unsplash.com/photos/low-angle-photo-brown-concrete-building-i5DMg-9hiZA" target="_blank">Mercedes Álvarez via Unsplash </a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Mural in the SoHo district of New York, one of the US cities featured in the study.</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; ֱ̽text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. Images, including our videos, are Copyright © ֱ̽ of Cambridge and licensors/contributors as identified.  All rights reserved. We make our image and video content available in a number of ways – as here, on our <a href="/">main website</a> under its <a href="/about-this-site/terms-and-conditions">Terms and conditions</a>, and on a <a href="/about-this-site/connect-with-us">range of channels including social media</a> that permit your use and sharing of our content under their respective Terms.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-license-type field-type-taxonomy-term-reference field-label-above"><div class="field-label">Licence type:&nbsp;</div><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/taxonomy/imagecredit/attribution">Attribution</a></div></div></div> Thu, 16 Dec 2021 10:15:40 +0000 fpjl2 228771 at ‘Wild West’ mentality lingers in US mountain regions /research/news/wild-west-mentality-lingers-in-us-mountain-regions <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/todd-diemer-z9qlecytmgq-unsplash.jpg?itok=Bu2hvNfD" alt="Man in a cowboy hat atop Humphreys Peak in Arizona, US" title="Man in a cowboy hat atop Humphreys Peak in Arizona, US, Credit: Todd Diemer" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>When historian Frederick Jackson Turner presented his famous thesis on the US frontier in 1893, <a href="https://wwnorton.com/college/history/archive/reader/trial/directory/1890_1914/ch21_frontier_thesis.htm">he described</a> the “coarseness and strength combined with acuteness and acquisitiveness” it had forged in the American character.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Now, well into the 21st century, and researchers led by the ֱ̽ of Cambridge have detected remnants of the pioneer personality in US populations of once inhospitable mountainous territory, particularly in the West.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>A team of scientists algorithmically investigated how landscape shapes psychology. They analysed links between the anonymised results of an online personality test completed by over 3.3 million Americans, and the “topography” of 37,227 US postal – or ZIP – codes.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽researchers found that living at both a higher altitude and an elevation relative to the surrounding region – indicating “hilliness” – is associated with a distinct blend of personality traits that fits with “frontier settlement theory”.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“ ֱ̽harsh and remote environment of mountainous frontier regions historically attracted nonconformist settlers strongly motivated by a sense of freedom,” said researcher Friedrich Götz, from Cambridge’s Department of Psychology.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Such rugged terrain likely favoured those who closely guarded their resources and distrusted strangers, as well as those who engaged in risky explorations to secure food and territory.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“These traits may have distilled over time into an individualism characterised by toughness and self-reliance that lies at the heart of the American frontier ethos” said Götz, lead author of the study.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“When we look at personality across the whole United States, we find that mountainous residents are more likely to have psychological characteristics indicative of this frontier mentality.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Götz worked with colleagues from the Karl Landsteiner ֱ̽ of Health Sciences, Austria, the ֱ̽ of Texas, US, the ֱ̽ of Melbourne in Australia, and his Cambridge supervisor Dr Jason Rentfrow. ֱ̽findings are published in the journal <a href="https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0930-x"><em>Nature Human Behaviour</em></a>.      </p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽research uses the “Big Five” personality model, standard in social psychology, with simple online tests providing high-to-low scores for five fundamental personality traits of millions of Americans.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽mix of characteristics uncovered by study’s authors consists of low levels of “agreeableness”, suggesting mountainous residents are less trusting and forgiving – traits that benefit “territorial, self-focused survival strategies”.   </p>&#13; &#13; <p>Low levels of “extraversion” reflect the introverted self-reliance required to thrive in secluded areas, and a low level of “conscientiousness” lends itself to rebelliousness and indifference to rules, say researchers.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Neuroticism” is also lower, suggesting an emotional stability and assertiveness suited to frontier living. However, “openness to experience” is much higher, and the most pronounced personality trait in mountain dwellers.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Openness is a strong predictor of residential mobility,” said Götz. “A willingness to move your life in pursuit of goals such as economic affluence and personal freedom drove many original North American frontier settlers.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Taken together, this psychological fingerprint for mountainous areas may be an echo of the personality types that sought new lives in unknown territories.”  </p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽researchers wanted to distinguish between the direct effects of physical environment and the “sociocultural influence” of growing up where frontier values and identities still hold sway.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>To do this, they looked at whether mountainous personality patterns applied to people born and raised in these regions that had since moved away.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽findings suggest some “initial enculturation” say researchers, as those who left their early mountain home are still consistently less agreeable, conscientious and extravert, although no such effects were observed for neuroticism and openness.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽scientists also divided the country at the edge of St. Louis – “gateway to the West” – to see if there is a personality difference between those in mountains that made up the historic frontier, such as the Rockies, and eastern ranges such as the Appalachians.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>While mountains continue to be a “meaningful predictor” of personality type on both sides of this divide, key differences emerged. Those in the east are more agreeable and outgoing, while western ranges are a closer fit for frontier settlement theory.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In fact, the mountainous effect on high levels of “openness to experience” is ten times as strong in residents of the old western frontier as in those of the eastern ranges.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽findings suggest that, while ecological effects are important, it is the lingering sociocultural effects – the stories, attitudes and education – in the former “Wild West” that are most powerful in shaping mountainous personality, according to scientists.   </p>&#13; &#13; <p>They describe the effect of mountain areas on personality as “small but robust”, but argue that complex psychological phenomena are influenced by many hundreds of factors, so small effects are to be expected.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Small effects can make a big difference at scale,” said Götz. “An increase of one standard deviation in mountainousness is associated with a change of around 1% in personality.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Over hundreds of thousands of people, such an increase would translate into highly consequential political, economic, social and health outcomes.”    </p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Distinct psychological mix associated with mountain populations is consistent with the theory that harsh frontiers attracted certain personalities. </p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">This psychological fingerprint for mountainous areas may be an echo of the personality types that sought new lives in unknown territories</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Friedrich Götz</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="https://unsplash.com/photos/man-standing-on-cliff-during-golden-hour-Z9QlecytmgQ" target="_blank">Todd Diemer</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Man in a cowboy hat atop Humphreys Peak in Arizona, US</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; ֱ̽text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. Images, including our videos, are Copyright © ֱ̽ of Cambridge and licensors/contributors as identified.  All rights reserved. We make our image and video content available in a number of ways – as here, on our <a href="/">main website</a> under its <a href="/about-this-site/terms-and-conditions">Terms and conditions</a>, and on a <a href="/about-this-site/connect-with-us">range of channels including social media</a> that permit your use and sharing of our content under their respective Terms.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-license-type field-type-taxonomy-term-reference field-label-above"><div class="field-label">Licence type:&nbsp;</div><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/taxonomy/imagecredit/attribution">Attribution</a></div></div></div> Mon, 07 Sep 2020 15:01:29 +0000 fpjl2 217602 at Industrial Revolution: damaging psychological ‘imprint’ persists in today’s populations /research/news/industrial-revolution-damaging-psychological-imprint-persists-in-todays-populations <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/m7jarbgt.jpg?itok=XpO7weJ4" alt="Industrial workplace" title="Industrial workplace, Credit: Wellcome Images" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>People living in the former industrial heartlands of England and Wales are more disposed to negative emotions such as anxiety and depressive moods, more impulsive and more likely to struggle with planning and self-motivation, according to a new study of almost 400,000 personality tests.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽findings show that, generations after the white heat of Industrial Revolution and decades on from the decline of deep coal mining, the populations of areas where coal-based industries dominated in the 19th century retain a “psychological adversity”. </p>&#13; &#13; <p>Researchers suggest this is the inherited product of selective migrations during mass industrialisation compounded by the social effects of severe work and living conditions.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>They argue that the damaging cognitive legacy of coal is “reinforced and amplified” by the more obvious economic consequences of high unemployment we see today. ֱ̽study also found significantly lower life satisfaction in these areas.   </p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽UK findings, published in the <a href="https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29154557/"><em>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</em></a>, are supported by a North American “robustness check”, with less detailed data from US demographics suggesting the same patterns of post-industrial personality traits. </p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Regional patterns of personality and well-being may have their roots in major societal changes underway decades or centuries earlier, and the Industrial Revolution is arguably one of the most influential and formative epochs in modern history,” says co-author Dr Jason Rentfrow, from Cambridge’s Department of Psychology.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Those who live in a post-industrial landscape still do so in the shadow of coal, internally as well as externally. This study is one of the first to show that the Industrial Revolution has a hidden psychological heritage, one that is imprinted on today’s psychological make-up of the regions of England and Wales.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>An international team of psychologists, including researchers from the Queensland ֱ̽ of Technology, ֱ̽ of Texas, ֱ̽ of Cambridge and the Baden-Wuerttemberg Cooperative State ֱ̽, used data collected from 381,916 people across England and Wales during 2009-2011 as part of the BBC Lab’s online Big Personality Test.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽team analysed test scores by looking at the “big five” personality traits: extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism and openness. ֱ̽results were further dissected by characteristics such as altruism, self-discipline and anxiety. </p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽data was also broken down by region and county, and compared with several other large-scale datasets including coalfield maps and a male occupation census of the early 19th century (collated through parish baptism records, where the father listed his job).</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽team controlled for an extensive range of other possible influences – from competing economic factors in the 19th century and earlier, through to modern considerations of education, wealth and even climate.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>However, they still found significant personality differences for those currently occupying areas where large numbers of men had been employed in coal-based industries from 1813 to 1820 – as the Industrial Revolution was peaking.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Neuroticism was, on average, 33% higher in these areas compared with the rest of the country. In the ‘big five’ model of personality, this translates as increased emotional instability, prone to feelings of worry or anger, as well as higher risk of common mental disorders such as depression and substance abuse.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In fact, in the further “sub-facet” analyses, these post-industrial areas scored 31% higher for tendencies toward both anxiety and depression.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Areas that ranked highest for neuroticism include Blaenau Gwent and Ceredigion in South Wales, and Hartlepool in England. </p>&#13; &#13; <p>Conscientiousness was, on average, 26% lower in former industrial areas. In the ‘big five’ model, this manifests as more disorderly and less goal-oriented behaviours – difficulty with planning and saving money. ֱ̽underlying sub-facet of ‘order’ itself was 35% lower in these areas.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽lowest three areas for conscientiousness were all in Wales (Merthyr Tydfil, Ceredigion and Gwynedd), with English areas including Nottingham and Leicester.   </p>&#13; &#13; <p>An assessment of life satisfaction was included in the BBC Lab questionnaire, which was an average of 29% lower in former industrial centres. </p>&#13; &#13; <p>While researchers say there will be many factors behind the correlation between personality traits and historic industrialisation, they offer two likely ones: migration and socialisation (learned behaviour).    </p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽people migrating into industrial areas were often doing so to find employment in the hope of escaping poverty and distressing situations of rural depression – those experiencing high levels of ‘psychological adversity’.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>However, people that left these areas, often later on, were likely those with higher degrees of optimism and psychological resilience, say researchers.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>This “selective influx and outflow” may have concentrated so-called ‘negative’ personality traits in industrial areas – traits that can be passed down generations through combinations of experience and genetics.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Migratory effects would have been exacerbated by the ‘socialisation’ of repetitive, dangerous and exhausting labour from childhood – reducing well-being and elevating stress – combined with harsh conditions of overcrowding and atrocious sanitation during the age of steam.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽study’s authors argue their findings have important implications for today’s policymakers looking at public health interventions.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“ ֱ̽decline of coal in areas dependent on such industries has caused persistent economic hardship – most prominently high unemployment. This is only likely to have contributed to the baseline of psychological adversity the Industrial Revolution imprinted on some populations,” says co-author Michael Stuetzer from Baden-Württemberg Cooperative State ֱ̽, Germany.   </p>&#13; &#13; <p>“These regional personality levels may have a long history, reaching back to the foundations of our industrial world, so it seems safe to assume they will continue to shape the well-being, health, and economic trajectories of these regions.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽team note that, while they focused on the negative psychological imprint of coal, future research could examine possible long-term positive effects in these regions born of the same adversity – such as the solidarity and civic engagement witnessed in the labour movement.  </p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Study finds people in areas historically reliant on coal-based industries have more ‘negative’ personality traits. Psychologists suggest this cognitive die may well have been cast at the dawn of the industrial age.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"> ֱ̽Industrial Revolution has a hidden psychological heritage, one that is imprinted on today’s psychological make-up of the regions of England and Wales</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Jason Rentfrow</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="https://wellcomecollection.org/works/m7jarbgt" target="_blank">Wellcome Images</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Industrial workplace</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; ֱ̽text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-license-type field-type-taxonomy-term-reference field-label-above"><div class="field-label">Licence type:&nbsp;</div><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/taxonomy/imagecredit/attribution">Attribution</a></div></div></div> Sun, 10 Dec 2017 09:22:56 +0000 fpjl2 193962 at Personality traits linked to differences in brain structure /research/news/personality-traits-linked-to-differences-in-brain-structure <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/145453165270d118ab1e4k.jpg?itok=i0cCO7MX" alt="Drain Brain Spider" title="Drain Brain Spider, Credit: darkday" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>According to psychologists, the extraordinary variety of human personality can be broken down into the so-called ‘Big Five’ personality traits, namely neuroticism (how moody a person is), extraversion (how enthusiastic a person is), openness (how open-minded a person is), agreeableness (a measure of altruism), and conscientiousness (a measure of self-control).</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In a study published today in the journal Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, an international team of researchers from the UK, US, and Italy have analysed a brain imaging dataset from over 500 individuals that has been made publicly available by the Human Connectome Project, a major US initiative funded by the National Institutes of Health. In particular, the researchers looked at differences in the brain cortical anatomy (the structure of the outer layer of the brain) as indexed by three measures – the thickness, area, and amount of folding in the cortex – and how these measures related to the Big Five personality traits.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Evolution has shaped our brain anatomy in a way that maximizes its area and folding at the expense of reduced thickness of the cortex,” explains Dr Luca Passamonti from the Department of Clinical Neurosciences at the ֱ̽ of Cambridge. “It’s like stretching and folding a rubber sheet – this increases the surface area, but at the same time the sheet itself becomes thinner. We refer to this as the ‘cortical stretching hypothesis’.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Cortical stretching is a key evolutionary mechanism that enabled human brains to expand rapidly while still fitting into our skulls, which grew at a slower rate than the brain,” adds Professor Antonio Terracciano from the Department of Geriatrics at the Florida State ֱ̽. “Interestingly, this same process occurs as we develop and grow in the womb and throughout childhood, adolescence, and into adulthood: the thickness of the cortex tends to decrease while the area and folding increase.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In addition, as we get older, neuroticism goes down – we become better at handling emotions. At the same time, conscientiousness and agreeableness go up – we become progressively more responsible and less antagonistic.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽researchers found that high levels of neuroticism, which may predispose people to develop neuropsychiatric disorders, were associated with increased thickness as well as reduced area and folding in some regions of the cortex such as the prefrontal-temporal cortices at the front of the brain.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In contrast, openness, which is a personality trait linked with curiosity, creativity and a preference for variety and novelty, was associated with the opposite pattern, reduced thickness and an increase in area and folding in some prefrontal cortices.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><img alt="" src="/sites/www.cam.ac.uk/files/inner-images/pictures_press_release.jpg" style="width: 100%; height: 100%;" />“Our work supports the notion that personality is, to some degree, associated with brain maturation, a developmental process that is strongly influenced by genetic factors,” says Dr Roberta Riccelli from Italy.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Of course, we are continually shaped by our experiences and environment, but the fact that we see clear differences in brain structure which are linked with differences in personality traits suggests that there will almost certainly be an element of genetics involved,” says Professor Nicola Toschi from the ֱ̽ ‘Tor Vergata’ in Rome. “This is also in keeping with the notion that differences in personality traits can be detected early on during development, for example in toddlers or infants.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽volunteers whose brains were imaged as part of the Human Connectome Project were all healthy individuals aged between 22 and 36 years with no history of neuro-psychiatric or other major medical problems. However, the relationship between differences in brain structure and personality traits in these people suggests that the differences may be even more pronounced in people who are more likely to experience neuro-psychiatric illnesses.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Linking how brain structure is related to basic personality traits is a crucial step to improving our understanding of the link between the brain morphology and particular mood, cognitive, or behavioural disorders,” adds Dr Passamonti. “We also need to have a better understanding of the relation between brain structure and function in healthy people to figure out what is different in people with neuropsychiatric disorders.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>This is not the first time the researchers have found links between our brain structure and behaviour. <a href="/research/news/map-of-teenage-brain-provides-strong-evidence-of-link-between-serious-antisocial-behaviour-and-brain">A study published by the group last year</a> found that the brains of teenagers with serious antisocial behaviour problems differ significantly in structure to those of their peers.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><em><strong>Reference</strong><br />&#13; Riccelli, R et al. <a href="https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw175">Surface-based morphometry reveals the neuroanatomical basis of the five-factor model.</a> Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience; 25 Jan 2016; DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsw175</em></p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Our personality may be shaped by how our brain works, but in fact the <em>shape </em>of our brain can itself provide surprising clues about how we behave – and our risk of developing mental health disorders – suggests a study published today.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Linking how brain structure is related to basic personality traits is a crucial step to improving our understanding of the link between the brain morphology and particular mood, cognitive, or behavioural disorders</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Luca Passamonti</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="https://www.flickr.com/photos/drainrat/14545316527/" target="_blank">darkday</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Drain Brain Spider</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; ֱ̽text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-license-type field-type-taxonomy-term-reference field-label-above"><div class="field-label">Licence type:&nbsp;</div><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/taxonomy/imagecredit/attribution">Attribution</a></div></div></div> Wed, 25 Jan 2017 00:01:16 +0000 cjb250 183872 at Biosocial science: ֱ̽murky history of the nature and nurture debate /research/discussion/biosocial-science-the-murky-history-of-the-nature-and-nurture-debate <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/news/discussion/convo2.jpg?itok=jDmKX-28" alt=" ֱ̽National Museum of Emerging Science and Innovation." title=" ֱ̽National Museum of Emerging Science and Innovation., Credit: MIKI Yoshihito" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>Self-righteousness, gratitude, sympathy, sincerity, and guilt – what if these social behaviours are biologically influenced, encoded within our genes and shaped by the forces of evolution to promote the survival of the human species? Does free will truly exist if our genes are inherited and our environment is a series of events set in motion before we are born?</p>&#13; &#13; <p>American biologist E O Wilson made these arguments when he published <a href="https://www.hup.harvard.edu/catalog.php?isbn=9780674002357">Sociobiology: ֱ̽New Synthesis</a> in 1975 and <a href="https://www.hup.harvard.edu/catalog.php?isbn=9780674016385&amp;amp;content=reviews">On Human Nature</a> in 1978. Wilson is the father of sociobiology, a field that believes social behaviour in animals, including humans, is biologically determined – partially shaped by genes and the forces of evolution. Time magazine picked up the emerging new scientific field, dedicating the <a href="https://content.time.com/time/covers/0,16641,19770801,00.html">August 1977 cover</a> to “Sociobiology: A New Theory of Behavior.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Today, it is a field still shrouded with controversy, but one which is offering new views on how our environment influences who we are and what we do.</p>&#13; &#13; <h2>Likened to eugenics</h2>&#13; &#13; <p>At its conception, sociobiology ignited <a href="https://www.nybooks.com/articles/1975/11/13/against-sociobiology/">heated criticism </a>from prominent biologists including Stephen Jay Gould and Robert Lewontin. They argued that the field was biologically determinist and perpetuated eugenic ideologies that sought to legitimise racial and social hierarchies. As critics pointed out, while “sociobiology” as a formal field did not come into existence until the 1970s, research that used biological explanations to justify social phenomena was not new.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>To figures such as Gould and Lewontin, this “biosocial” scientific language lived in the fields of physical anthropology and eugenics. In the early 20th century, eugenicists like Madison Grant had used this kind of language to <a href="https://www.jstor.org/stable/207903?seq=1">explain</a> and justify class and race hierarchies. Supporters of such ideas used them to <a href="https://books.google.com/books/about/Study_of_American_Intelligence.html?id=IGxEAAAAYAAJcitation">advocate</a> for social policies prohibiting class and racial mixing, and restrictions on immigration.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Biosocial science was soon used as a guise for the eugenics movement. ֱ̽American Eugenics Society changed its name in 1972 to the Society for the Study of Social Biology, three years before the field of “sociobiology” was formally established. ֱ̽society’s official journal Eugenics Quarterly, whose first volume in 1954 focused heavily on IQ differences between population groups, changed its name to Social Biology in 1969. It continues to exist today under the name of <a href="https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hsbi20">Biodemography and Social Biology</a>.</p>&#13; &#13; <h2>Social life in ‘molecular terms’</h2>&#13; &#13; <p>Sociobiology has also influenced the development of “sociogenomics” – a term coined in 2005 by molecular biologist Gene Robinson whose <a href="https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/342901?seq=1">work</a> examines the genetic mechanisms governing social behaviour in the honeybee. Though early sociogenomics work focused primarily on insect populations, the field has moved to include an examination of human populations.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Sociogenomics is a field driven by two desires. ֱ̽first is to identify the genes and pathways that regulate aspects of development, physiology and behaviour that in turn influence the way animals or humans develop social links and form cooperative communities. ֱ̽second is to determine how these genes and pathways themselves are <a href="https://www.nature.com/articles/nrg1575">influenced by social life</a> and social evolution. Yet in practice, these two main components of sociogenomics research seem to be in conflict.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>One side tries to identify genetic markers associated with behaviours commonly thought to be shaped by social interactions. Researchers have looked at everything from <a href="https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0168952512001114">political orientation</a> to <a href="https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.1235488">educational attainment</a> and antisocial behaviour <a href="https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00224540903366503">linked to criminality</a>.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Some studies have <a href="https://www.cell.com/current-biology/abstract/S0960-9822(16)31119-8">sought</a> to find genetic variations linked to social phenomena like social deprivation and household income. One <a href="https://www.cell.com/current-biology/abstract/S0960-9822(16)31119-8">study</a> claimed to have identified common genetic variations that can explain up to 21% of the observed differences in social deprivation between individuals.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Such research has, however, garnered some more recent criticism from researchers critical of the <a href="https://books.google.co.uk/books/about/The_Trouble_with_Twin_Studies.html?id=AkCLBQAAQBAJ&amp;source=kp_cover&amp;redir_esc=y&amp;hl=en">underlying methods</a> used and the field’s <a href="https://books.google.co.uk/books/about/Misbehaving_Science.html?id=9bHBAwAAQBAJ&amp;source=kp_cover&amp;redir_esc=y">ethical implications</a>.</p>&#13; &#13; <h2>Nature and nurture</h2>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽other side of sociogenomics examines how the environment moderates what’s called “gene expression”. This is the process by which genes are “activated” to synthesise proteins that allow the genotype (an individual’s genetic makeup) to give rise to a phenotype (an observed behaviour or trait).</p>&#13; &#13; <figure class="align-center "><img alt="" src="https://62e528761d0685343e1c-f3d1b99a743ffa4142d9d7f1978d9686.ssl.cf2.rackcdn.com/files/153266/width754/image-20170118-26577-8pmy0w.jpg" style="height: 565px; width: 565px;" /><figcaption><span class="caption">Not just one or the other.</span> <span class="attribution"><span class="source">Stuart Miles/shutterstock.com</span></span></figcaption></figure><p>In this form of sociogenomics, the classical argument of “nature versus nurture” becomes more clearly a matter of both “nature and nurture”. Social or environmental conditions such as low social status, social isolation or low socioeconomic status have been found to change the expression of hundreds of genes in both <a href="https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.aah3580">animals</a> and humans.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>This is now considered by some to be potentially <a href="https://livestream.com/baystateeventsolutions/tannerlectures2?t=1478203316">transformative</a> in our approach to addressing inequality. For example, biosocial research which shows how structural or environmental aspects influence biological processes could throw much needed weight behind socially-oriented policies. On the other hand, biosocial researchers might argue that rather than fix what’s happening in society, we could focus on trying to treat biological deficits.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Gene x environment” studies, as they are called, have <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x">found</a> that in the US, low socioeconomic status represses an individual’s genetic potential. This means, for example, that the high estimates for genetic influence on educational attainment may only fully apply to those living in well-off circumstances, where money, status, and comfort are not pressing concerns.</p>&#13; &#13; <h2>Mixing the hard and social sciences</h2>&#13; &#13; <p><a href="https://press.princeton.edu/titles/10801.html">Some advocates</a> for the biosocial sciences believe the social sciences will become more robust and more highly regarded with the incorporation of genetics research. There are sociologists, economists, and political scientists who are already beginning to bring genetic analyses into their work. They <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0094306114539640">argue that</a> this additional data may help the social sciences “better understand patterns of human behavior, enhance individuals’ self-understanding, and design optimal public policy”.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Such mixing of the traditionally hard and social sciences has produced studies in sociogenomics <a href="https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0050576">examining</a> how high taxation of tobacco products meant to discourage people from purchasing harmful products may not be beneficial for those with a particular variant of the nicotine receptor that might make them willing to pay more for tobacco. It has also contributed to research looking at cortisol levels in young ethnic-minorities as they note racism or discrimination. This <a href="https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0306453014003436">work has highlighted</a> how everyday micro-aggressions and social inequality can have real and harmful biological consequences.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>These studies point to the continued desire to explain social phenomena through biology. As the biosocial sciences continue the journey to analyse everyday human life and behaviour, they have the potential to have a profound impact – both positive and negative – on our understandings of how we as individuals and we as a society operate.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><span><a href="https://theconversation.com/profiles/daphne-martschenko-238687">Daphne Martschenko</a>, PhD Candidate, <em><a href="https://theconversation.com/institutions/university-of-cambridge-1283"> ֱ̽ of Cambridge</a></em></span></p>&#13; &#13; <p>This article was originally published on <a href="https://theconversation.com/"> ֱ̽Conversation</a>. Read the <a href="https://theconversation.com/biosocial-science-the-murky-history-of-the-nature-and-nurture-debate-70028">original article</a>.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p> ֱ̽idea that social behaviours are biologically influenced is controversial, but may provide new views on how our environment influences who we are and what we do, writes Daphne Martschenko from the Faculty of Education.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="https://theconversation.com/biosocial-science-the-murky-history-of-the-nature-and-nurture-debate-70028" target="_blank">MIKI Yoshihito</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"> ֱ̽National Museum of Emerging Science and Innovation.</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; ֱ̽text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div> Mon, 23 Jan 2017 09:19:53 +0000 ljm67 183852 at Spending for smiles: money can buy happiness after all /research/news/spending-for-smiles-money-can-buy-happiness-after-all <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/pic_6.png?itok=5VF7xzX4" alt="Shopping" title="Shopping, Credit: Glen Scott" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>People who spent more money on purchases which matched their personality were happier, found the <a href="https://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2016/04/05/0956797616635200.abstract" target="_blank">study</a>, published in the journal <em>Psychological Science</em>. According to the researchers, matching spending with personality was more important for individuals’ happiness than the effect of individuals’ total income or their total spending.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽study, by researchers from the ֱ̽ of Cambridge, was conducted in collaboration with a UK-based multinational bank. Customers were asked whether they would complete a standard personality and happiness questionnaire, and to consent to their responses being matched anonymously for research purposes with their bank transaction data.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽final study was based on 76,863 transactions of 625 participants. ֱ̽study whittled down 112 spending categories automatically grouped by the bank into 59 categories that had at least 500 transactions over a six-month period.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽study matched spending categories on the widely recognised “Big Five” personality traits – openness to experience (artistic versus traditional), conscientiousness (self-controlled vs easy-going), extraversion (outgoing vs reserved), agreeableness (compassionate vs competitive), and neuroticism (prone to stress vs stable).</p>&#13; &#13; <p>For example, eating out in pubs was rated as an extroverted and low conscientiousness (impulsive) spending category, whereas charities and pets were rated as agreeable spending categories. Further examples can be found below.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽researchers then compared the participants’ actual purchases to their personalities using this scale, and found that people generally spent more money on products that match their personality. For example, a highly extroverted person spent approximately £52 more each year on pub nights than an introverted person. Similarly, a highly conscientiousness person spent £124 more annually on health and fitness than a person low in conscientiousness.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽study was authored by Sandra Matz, a PhD candidate in Cambridge’s Department of Psychology; Joe Gladstone, a Research Associate at Cambridge Judge Business School; and David Stillwell, ֱ̽ Lecturer in Big Data Analytics &amp; Quantitative Social Science at Cambridge Judge Business School.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Historically, studies had found a weak relationship between money and overall wellbeing,” said Gladstone. “Our study breaks new ground by mining actual bank transaction data and demonstrating that spending can increase our happiness when it is spent on goods and services that fit our personalities and so meet our psychological needs.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽researchers believe the findings hold widespread implications, including for Internet businesses using search-based recommendation engines. Companies can use this information to recommend products and services that don’t just increase clicks, but will actually improve the wellbeing of their customers – allowing companies to forge better relationships with customers based on what makes them happier.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> ֱ̽researchers also backed up their findings by running a second experiment, where they gave people a voucher to spend either in a bookshop or at a bar. Extroverts who were forced to spend at a bar were happier than introverts forced to spend at a bar, while introverts forced to spend at a bookshop were happier than extroverts forced to spend at a bookshop. This follow-up experiment overcomes the limitations of correlational data by demonstrating that spending money on things that match a person’s personality can cause an increase in happiness.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>“Our findings suggest that spending money on products that help us express who we are as individuals could turn out to be as important to our well-being as finding the right job, the right neighbourhood or even the right friends and partners,” said Matz. “By developing a more nuanced understanding of the links between spending and happiness, we hope to be able to provide more personalised advice on how to find happiness through the little consumption choices we make every day.”</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Categories with the lowest and highest scores on each of the Big Five personality traits:</p>&#13; &#13; <table border="1" cellpadding="1" cellspacing="1" style="width:500px;"><tbody><tr><td><strong>Big 5 Trait    </strong>  </td>&#13; <td><strong>Low</strong></td>&#13; <td><strong>High</strong></td>&#13; </tr><tr><td>Openness </td>&#13; <td>Traffic fines, residential mortgages</td>&#13; <td>Entertainment, hair and beauty</td>&#13; </tr><tr><td>Conscientiousness</td>&#13; <td>Gambling, toys and hobbies  </td>&#13; <td>Home insurance, health, fitness</td>&#13; </tr><tr><td>Extraversion</td>&#13; <td>Home insurance, accountant fees</td>&#13; <td>Entertainment, travel</td>&#13; </tr><tr><td>Agreeableness </td>&#13; <td>Traffic fines, gambling</td>&#13; <td>Charities, pets</td>&#13; </tr><tr><td>Neuroticism</td>&#13; <td>Stationery, hotels </td>&#13; <td>Traffic fines, gambling</td>&#13; </tr></tbody></table><p><em><strong>Reference:</strong><br />&#13; Sandra C. Matz, Joe J. Gladstone, and David Stillwell. ‘<a href="https://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2016/04/05/0956797616635200.abstract" target="_blank">Money Buys Happiness When Spending Fits Our Personality</a>.’ Psychological Science (2016). DOI: 10.1177/0956797616635200</em></p>&#13; &#13; <p><em>Adapted from a Cambridge Judge Business School <a href="https://www.jbs.cam.ac.uk/insight/">press release</a>.  </em></p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Money really can buy happiness when spending fits our personality, finds a study based on 77,000 UK bank transactions.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Spending can increase our happiness when it is spent on goods and services that fit our personalities and so meet our psychological needs.</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Joe Gladstone</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="https://www.flickr.com/photos/glenscott/3892725795/in/photolist-6VZeKZ-28FfaU-dYqYaa-9We757-aipaCc-6ihZDq-7ekjo8-cAHzUA-dYwFs1-MjUVG-gqXpk7-7SHaTy-bQmtst-an88Mv-dS8eZc-aby2Qa-5AyVhb-7hnvyb-5meURW-MjUPy-MjUFy-57p1sR-7YqRRx-gfKNx-fQdcSR-dPTStw-gfKMY-f8xUrp-8Gs7cP-qEAzG-ieFspY-5YB47R-97rF6L-4GWVgG-6P25Hh-aA5jxk-nsP5Pu-9Mgb8Q-9y5UYs-bbp4Cc-aFnHrP-edMb5C-9gbUA7-Mk6gF-swgoP-4cag5E-pMqZ4F-4DNUw7-9zgJtC-aeZ1kk" target="_blank">Glen Scott</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Shopping</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; ֱ̽text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-license-type field-type-taxonomy-term-reference field-label-above"><div class="field-label">Licence type:&nbsp;</div><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/taxonomy/imagecredit/attribution-noncommerical">Attribution-Noncommerical</a></div></div></div> Thu, 07 Apr 2016 15:00:00 +0000 Anonymous 170922 at Opinion: What your musical taste says about your personality /research/discussion/opinion-what-your-musical-taste-says-about-your-personality <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/discussion/151130headphones.jpg?itok=Tk1epgLS" alt="Headphones" title="Headphones, Credit: Jake Bellucci" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>We’re exposed to music for nearly 20% of <a href="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00434.x/abstract">our waking lives</a>. But much of our musical experience seems to be a mystery. Why does some music bring us to tears while other pieces make us dance? Why is it that the music that we like can make others agitated? And why do some people seem to have a natural ability to play music while others have difficulty carrying a tune? Science is beginning to show that these individual differences are not just random but are, in part, due to people’s personalities.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>My colleagues and I <a href="https://journals.plos.org:443/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0131151">have published</a> research showing that people’s musical preferences are linked to three broad thinking styles. Empathisers (Type E) have a strong interest in people’s thoughts and emotions. Systemisers (Type S) have a strong interest in patterns, systems and the rules that govern the world. And those who score relatively equally on empathy and systemising are classified as Type B for “balanced”.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Research from the <a href="https://journals.plos.org:443/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0102251">past decade</a> has shown that 95% of people can be classified into one of these three groups and that they predict a lot of human behaviour. For example, they can predict things such as whether someone studies maths and science, or humanities <a href="https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1041608007000155">at university</a>. For the first time, we have shown that they can predict musical behaviour, too.</p>&#13; &#13; <h2>Matching music with thinking style</h2>&#13; &#13; <p>To study this phenomenon, we conducted <a href="https://journals.plos.org:443/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0131151">multiple studies</a> with over 4,000 participants. We took data on these participants’ thinking styles and asked them to listen to and indicate their preferences for up to 50 musical excerpts, representing a wide range of genres. Across these studies, we found that empathisers preferred mellow music that had low energy, sad emotions, and emotional depth, as heard in R&amp;B, soft rock, and singer-songwriter genres. For example, empathising was linked to preferences for “Come Away With Me” by Norah Jones and Jeff Buckley’s recording of “Hallelujah”.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> </p>&#13; &#13; <figure><iframe allowfullscreen="" frameborder="0" height="260" src="https://www.youtube.com/embed/y8AWFf7EAc4?wmode=transparent&amp;start=0" width="440"></iframe></figure><p> </p>&#13; &#13; <p>On the other hand, systemisers preferred more intense music, as heard in hard rock, punk and heavy metal genres. Systemisers also preferred music with intellectual depth and complexity as heard in avant-garde classical genres. For example, systemizing was linked to preferences for Alexander Scriabin’s “Etude opus 65 no 3”. Importantly, those who are Type B, had a tendency to prefer music that spans more of a range than the other two thinking styles.</p>&#13; &#13; <p> </p>&#13; &#13; <figure><iframe allowfullscreen="" frameborder="0" height="260" src="https://www.youtube.com/embed/pPvfq5H8PgQ?wmode=transparent&amp;start=0" width="440"></iframe></figure><p> </p>&#13; &#13; <p>In our <a href="http://www.doc.gold.ac.uk/~mas03dm/papers/Greenbergetal_PersonalityMusicalSophistication_2015.pdf">most recent study</a>, published in the Journal of Research of Personality, we found that people’s personality traits can also predict their musical ability, even if they don’t play an instrument. Our team worked with BBC Lab UK to recruit over 7,000 participants and assess them for five distinct personality dimensions: openness, conscientiousness, extroversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism/emotionality stability. We also asked them to conduct various tasks that measured their musical ability, including remembering melodies and picking out rhythms.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>We found that, next to musical training, the personality trait of openness was the strongest predictor of musical sophistication. People who score highly for openness are imaginative, have a wide range of interests, and are open to new ways of thinking and changes in their environment. Those who score low on openness (or who are “closed”) are more set in their ways, prefer routine and the familiar, and tend to have more conventional values. We also found that extroverts who are often more talkative, assertive, and excitement-seeking had greater singing abilities.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Furthermore, we could apply this even to people who did not currently play a musical instrument, meaning there are people who have a potential for musical talent but are entirely unaware of it.</p>&#13; &#13; <h2>Music therapy</h2>&#13; &#13; <p>These new findings tell us that from a person’s musical taste and ability, we can infer a range of information about their personality and the way that they think.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>This research shows there are factors beyond our awareness that shape our musical experiences. We hope that these findings can be of help to teachers, parents, and clinicians. Based on information about personality, educators can ensure that children with the potential for musical talent have the opportunity to learn a musical instrument. Music therapists can use information about thinking style to help tailor their therapies for clients, too.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>We are also interested in how knowledge gained from science can help children and adults on the autism spectrum who have difficulties with communication, as we recently wrote in the journal <a href="http://emusicology.org/article/view/4603">Empirical Musicology Review</a>. This could also help people process emotions after experiencing a psychological trauma and when grieving a loss. In fact, initial findings from our lab suggest that people who experienced a traumatic event in childhood engage with music quite differently in adulthood than those who did not experience a trauma.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><em>If you want to find out how you score on musical ability, preferences, and personality, you can take these tests at <a href="https://musicaluniverse.io/">www.musicaluniverse.org</a>.</em></p>&#13; &#13; <p><em><strong><span><a href="https://theconversation.com/profiles/david-m-greenberg-204317">David Greenberg</a>, PhD candidate, psychology, <a href="https://theconversation.com/institutions/university-of-cambridge-1283"> ֱ̽ of Cambridge</a></span></strong></em></p>&#13; &#13; <p><em><strong>This article was originally published on <a href="https://theconversation.com/"> ֱ̽Conversation</a>. Read the <a href="https://theconversation.com/what-your-musical-taste-says-about-your-personality-50492">original article</a>.</strong></em></p>&#13; &#13; <p><em> ֱ̽opinions expressed in this article are those of the individual author(s) and do not represent the views of the ֱ̽ of Cambridge.</em></p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>David Greenberg (Department of Psychology) discusses how musical preferences are linked to thinking styles.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="https://www.flickr.com/photos/jakescreations/69953132/in/photolist-7bwCu-nFS9i-4AxJJ8-ajxehR-6uzLyN-85JmbR-6SaZxn-9EcvFd-dRgtid-cwojCE-6FppEQ-23PhGJ-5UPajj-3wtLvH-97yhR9-futssE-6SStz9-qgsSmJ-9UTLtC-5nmEWP-4yFWkV-26dZo5-aEJeeH-dXqp7o-7KgDdP-mKjEYS-of9sHy-ouwR5F-5bxcwv-pA8s75-fYci6X-2hYGMj-5V2qF2-vY16-fvbKPX-cgVsy7-5LUJok-8EYNAC-9MY1Gf-akBP6s-buprei-cxbPvS-baFQGB-4iJJEk-5WsnPW-32mn8S-fAcvjc-dUuhoN-2dZvSS-9xRJvS" target="_blank">Jake Bellucci</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Headphones</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; ֱ̽text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-license-type field-type-taxonomy-term-reference field-label-above"><div class="field-label">Licence type:&nbsp;</div><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/taxonomy/imagecredit/attribution-noncommerical">Attribution-Noncommerical</a></div></div></div> Mon, 30 Nov 2015 01:40:32 +0000 Anonymous 163442 at